Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Preventive physiotherapy interventions for back care in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis

Abstract

Background

Preventive interventions improve healthy behaviours and they also increase knowledge regarding back care in children and adolescents, but studies exhibit great variability in their contents, duration and number of sessions, and in the assessment methods. The purpose of this study was to review the empirical evidence regarding preventive physiotherapy interventions for back care in children and adolescents, and to ascertain the most efficacious treatments, in what way and under which circumstances.

Methods

Studies were located from computerized databases (Cochrane Library, Medline, PEDro, Web of Science and IME) and other sources. The search period extended to May 2012. To be included in the meta-analysis, studies had to use physical therapy methodologies of preventive treatment on children and adolescents, and to compare a treatment and a control group. Treatment, participant, methodological, and extrinsic characteristics of the studies were coded. Two researchers independently coded all of the studies. As effect size indices, standardized mean differences were calculated for measures of behaviours and knowledge, both in the posttest and in the follow-up. The random and mixed-effects models were used for the statistical analyses and sensitivity analyses were carried out in order to check the robustness of the meta-analytic results.

Results

A total of 19 papers fulfilled the selection criteria, producing 23 independent studies. On average, the treatments reached a statistically significant effectiveness in the behaviours acquired, both in the posttest and in the follow-up (d + = 1.33 and d + = 1.80, respectively), as well as in measures of knowledge (posttest; d + = 1.29; follow-up: d + = 0.76). Depending on the outcome measure, the effect sizes were affected by different moderator variables, such as the type of treatment, the type of postural hygiene, the teaching method, or the use of paraprofessionals as cotherapists.

Conclusions

The interventions were successful in significantly increasing the behaviours and knowledge acquired both in the posttest and in the follow-up. The combined treatment of postural hygiene with physiotherapy exercise exhibited the best results. The small number of studies limits the generalizability of the results.

Peer Review reports

Background

Epidemiological studies indicate that non-specific low back pain (LBP) is already present during childhood[1] and it is one of the main reasons for suffering chronic LBP as an adult[2]. Many studies have shown a great prevalence of LBP in children and adolescents[37]. According to the literature on the epidemiology of LBP in children and adolescents, estimates of the lifetime prevalence vary between 8.6%[8] and 58.90%[6].

The risk of developing LBP depends on several factors[9]. Lifestyle-related factors, anthropometric factors, school-related factors and psychosocial factors are all associated with LBP in children and adolescents[2, 10, 11]. Programs for the prevention of LBP and discomfort have mainly been carried out on the adult population[1215], fundamentally due to the associated expenses that this disorder generates. In recent decades, as a result of the increase in morbidity of back problems in children and adolescents, a need has been detected to develop preventive interventions for this population group[16]. In answer to this need, the European Region of the World Confederation for Physical Therapy (ER-WCPT) has recently published the results of a study carried out in the European Union[17]. There is evidence that the preventive approach produces an increase in the acquisition of knowledge and an improvement in appropriate postural habits that favour back care in children and adolescents[1821].

Preventive interventions for children and adolescents have been aimed at increasing the cognitions related to protecting the back in everyday activities (at school, at home, and in sports) through different methods of teaching and learning[22, 23]. The preventive interventions that have been employed include physical therapy exercises to improve physical fitness[19, 20, 24], training in positions and movements used in everyday activities for a healthier back (to avoid overloading)[18, 2529] and, more recently, increasing physical activity[17, 30].

The school has been the location for numerous research studies on the development of back care interventions in this population[22, 25, 26, 31, 32], although interventions have varied considerably in many aspects, such as the type of intervention, teaching techniques, duration, magnitude and intensity of sessions, mode of intervention, characteristics of the participants, and how the interventions are assessed.

Due to the lack of previous meta-analyses in this context, our main objective was to assess the evidence regarding preventive physiotherapy interventions for back care in children and adolescents, and to ascertain which ones prove to be the most efficacious, in what way and under which circumstances. Our specific objectives were: (a) to estimate the efficacy of preventive physiotherapy interventions for back care in children and adolescents, and (b) to examine the influence of treatment, participant, methodological, contextual, and extrinsic characteristics of the studies on the effect size.

Starting from the literature on this subject, several hypotheses were formulated: (a) the intensity, magnitude and duration of treatment will be positively related to the results; (b) treatments that include external agents will obtain better results; (c) treatments that include the parents or teachers will attain greater effect sizes; (d) the sex of participants will influence results, in that girls will acquire greater knowledge than boys[33, 34]; (e) the age of participants will influence the effect sizes, greater knowledge being expected amongst older children and knowledge being improved when behaviours are acquired from a younger age, and (f) the type of control group has an influence on the effect size, as studies with a nonactive control group will produce higher effect sizes than studies with an active control group.

Methods

Study selection

The studies had to fulfil the following criteria to be selected: (a) the study had to apply some physical therapy methodology of preventive treatment for LBP; (b) the participants in the study had to pertain to a nonclinical population of children and/or adolescents aged below 19 years; (c) the study had to include, at least, a treatment and a control group; (d) the minimum sample size in the posttest had to be of 5 subjects per group; (e) the study had to report enough statistical data to calculate the effect sizes; (f) the study had to be published or carried out before May 2012; (g) the study might be written in English, Spanish, French, Italian, Portuguese, and Catalan. Finally, studies in which all subjects in the sample presented pain, spinal diseases or surgical vertebral treatment were excluded, since our focus was on preventive interventions.

Data sources and searches

Combined search processes were used for locating the studies, clearly planned and ordered. The following specialized bibliographical databases were consulted: the Cochrane Library, Medline, PEDro, Web of Science and IME (Spanish Medical Index). The search period extended to May 2012, with the following key words: children, adolescents, treatment, prevention, education, “postural hygiene”, “physical education”, “back education”, “posture education”, “back function”, physiotherapy, ergonomics, ”physical therapy”, “exercise therapy”, promotion, behaviour, “back care”, “back pain”, “low back pain”. For details regarding the search terms and combinations, see Additional file1. Journals from the Elsevier Iberoamerican database were also consulted, as well as specialized electronic journals. References of relevant papers already identified were consulted and, in order to locate unpublished studies, letters were sent to experts in the field and congress acts and doctoral theses were consulted.

A total of 956 references were located, from which 905 were excluded in a first screening. The main reasons for deleting these studies were because the participants in the samples were adults (about 50%) or pertained to clinical populations, such as diseases that cause back pain (about 15%), because of applying pharmacological treatments for LBP (about 20%), or by other reasons (about 15%). The reading of the remaining 62 papers allowed us to identify 19 articles that fulfilled the selection criteria. The Additional file2 presents the flow chart of the selection process of the studies. Given that some papers included two groups that were receiving alternative treatments and a control group, a total of 23 studies were included, with a study being defined as a comparison between a treatment and a control group.

Data extraction and quality assessment

In order to assure the maximum possible objectivity, a codebook was produced that specified the standards followed in coding each of the characteristics of the studies. The moderator variables of the 23 studies were coded and grouped into three categories according to Lipsey’s recommendations[35]: substantive (treatment, context and participant), methodological, and extrinsic variables.

The following treatment characteristics were coded: (a) the type of preventive physiotherapy treatment (postural hygiene, physiotherapy exercise, physical activity); (b) the acquisition mode of postural hygiene (acquisition of knowledge, posture training habits); (c) the teaching method of postural hygiene (theoretical, practical); (d) the type of physiotherapy exercise (stretching, strengthening, pelvic tilt exercises, breathing, posture correction, balance exercises); (e) the type of physical activity (sports, games); (f) the duration of the treatment (in weeks); (g) the intensity of the treatment (number of weekly hours of treatment received by each subject); (h) the magnitude of the treatment (total number of hours received by each subject); (i) the existence of an established number of sessions; (j) the homogeneity of the treatment (whether all patients received the treatment in the same conditions; (k) the inclusion of homework; (l) the inclusion of a follow-up program; (m) the use of external agents to the therapeutic group (subjects that are not part of the therapy group, who are not professionals, but who have an influence, being able to support the subjects in attaining their therapy goals); (n) the presence of family members who act as cotherapists that continue or carry out preventive treatment at home); (o) the presence of teachers who act as cotherapists that continue or carry out preventive treatment at home; (p) the mode of application of the intervention (direct, indirect or mixed); (q) the mode of training (group, individual or mixed); (r) the use of informed consent. Regarding the characteristics of the therapists the following variables were coded: (s) the number of therapists; (t) whether or not the authors agree with the therapists; (u) the training of the therapist (physiotherapist, other); (v) the experience of the therapists (large, medium, low, mixed), and (w) the gender of therapists (men, women, mixed).

The participant characteristics coded in the samples of each study were: (a) the mean age of the subjects (in years); (b) the gender of the sample (percentage of males); (c) the physical activity level of subjects during the intervention (low, moderate, regular), and (d) whether or not they had undertaken previous treatments. Only two contextual characteristics were coded: (a) the country and (b) the place where the intervention was carried out (university, clinic, health centre / day centre, hospital, school, sports centre, mixed).

The following methodological characteristics were coded: (a) whether pretest measures were used; (b) how the subjects were allocated to the treatments (randomly vs. nonrandomly); (c) the type of control group (nonactive vs. active); (d) the largest follow-up in the study (in months); (e) the sample size; (f) the attrition in the posttest; (g) the attrition in the follow-up; (h) the methodological quality of the study measured on a scale of 0 to 8 points following van Tulder[36] but with a few adaptations to our selected studies (the scale consisted of adding the scores of eight items: random assignment, control group type, sample size, attrition, intent-to-treat analysis, evaluator blinding, homogeneous assessment, and inter-rater reliability).

Finally, the extrinsic characteristics coded were: (a) the year of the study; (b) the profession of the first author (physiotherapist, ergonomist, teacher, physician, other) and (c) the publication source (published vs. unpublished). In addition, given that the studies included in the meta-analysis came from a few research teams, this characteristic was also coded in order to examine its potential influence on the study results.

In order to assess the inter-coder reliability of the coding process, two researchers (A.G.C. and I.C.M.) independently coded all of the studies. For the quantitative moderator variables intra-class correlation coefficients were calculated (ICC), while for the qualitative moderator variables Cohen’s kappa coefficients were applied. On average, the ICC was 0.995 (range: 0.954 to 1) and the kappa coefficient was 1, which were highly satisfactory, as proposed by Orwin and Vevea[37]. The inconsistencies between the coders were solved by consensus and the coding manual was corrected when the cause of these inconsistencies was due to an error in it. The codebook can be obtained from the corresponding author.

Effect size index

The standardized mean difference, d[38], was used as the effect size index, adhering to the following definitions according to whether or not the study included pretest measurements: when the study did not include pretest measurements, a standardized mean difference was calculated, defined as the difference between the treatment and control means in the posttest, divided by a pooled within-group standard deviation. The same index was applied for the follow-up measurements. When the study included pretest measurements, the effect size index was the standardized mean change[39], defined as the difference between the pretest-posttest mean change for the treatment and control groups, divided by a pooled estimate of the pretest standard deviations of the two groups. Similar effect sizes were calculated from the pretest-follow-up measurements. Four studies compared two alternative treatments with the same control group, so that the data from the control group were used twice in the effect size calculations[22, 27, 40, 41]. In order to minimize the dependence produced by sharing the control group[42], its sample size was divided in two.

Separate effect sizes were calculated for two different outcomes: behaviours and knowledge measurements. Thus, from each study four effect sizes might be calculated: behaviours and knowledge in the posttest, and behaviours and knowledge in the follow-up. The effect sizes were calculated from means, standard deviations and other statistics, such as T-tests, F-tests, etc.[43, 44]. In order to check the reliability of the effect size calculations, two independent researchers (J.S.M and I.C.M) carried out the calculations for all of the studies, reaching an average intra-class correlation coefficient of 0.950 (range: 0.701-1), which were also highly satisfactory[37].

Data analysis

With the effect sizes obtained for behaviours and knowledge, both in the posttest and in the follow-up, separate meta-analyses were carried out. This implied to construct a forest plot, to obtain a mean effect size with its 95% confidence interval, and to assess the effect sizes’ heterogeneity with the Q statistic and the I 2 index[45]. For these calculations, a random-effects model was applied and this implied to weight each effect size by its inverse-variance, with the variance defined as the sum of the within-study and the between-studies variances[38]. Sensitivity analyses were carried out in order to assess the robustness of the meta-analytic results. Thus, the influence of outlying effect sizes was assessed by deleting them from the statistical analyses, in order to check whether a few data might affect the results. In addition, funnel plots were constructed and the trim-and-fill method[46] was applied, to assess whether publication bias might be a threat to the validity of the meta-analytic results. When the meta-analysis included, at least, 10 studies, the influence of moderator variables was checked by applying mixed-effects analyses of variance (ANOVAs), for the qualitative variables, and simple meta-regressions, for the quantitative ones. In the ANOVAs and meta-regressions, Q B and Q R statistics were calculated, respectively, to assess the statistical significance of the moderator variables, and Q W and Q E statistics to assess the model misspecification. To estimate the effect magnitude of each moderator variable on the effect sizes, the proportion of variance accounted for proposed by Raudenbush[47] was applied: R 2 = 1 τ ^ Res 2 / τ ^ Total 2 , with τ ^ Total 2 and τ ^ Res 2 being the total and residual between-studies variances, respectively[38]. The statistical analyses were made using the meta-analysis macros developed by David B. Wilson for the statistical package SPSS[48]. The forest plots were carried out with RevMan 5.1[49], and the funnel plots with the trim-and-fill method were obtained from the package Comprehensive Meta-analysis 2.0[50]. The PRISMA checklist[51] was used to check the reporting quality of the meta-analysis (Additional file3).

Results

Descriptive characteristics of the studies

Nineteen papers fulfilled the selection criteria[1924, 2730, 33, 34, 40, 41, 5256], generating a total of 23 independent studies each one comparing a treatment and a control group. The studies were carried out between 1984 and 2011, two of them being unpublished papers (Additional file2). Eleven studies were carried out in Belgium, seven in Spain, two in USA and South Korea and one in Brazil. In the pretest, a total of 4,519 participants were distributed into 21 treatment groups (2,554 participants; median: 44) and 16 control groups (1,965 participants; median: 50). In the posttest, the total sample size was of 4,423 participants, 2,493 pertaining to the treatment groups (median: 42) and 1,930 to the control groups (median: 50). In the follow-up, the total sample size reduced to 2,605 participants, distributed into 12 treatment groups (1,344 participants; median: 84) and 11 control groups (1,261 participants; median: 98).

The individual characteristics of each of the integrated studies are presented in Table 1. In relation to the type of intervention, the most noteworthy was postural hygiene applied on its own (19 studies), in comparison with the combined treatment of postural hygiene and physical therapy exercises (three studies) and postural hygiene and physical activity (one study). The median number of weeks of intervention was 6, the median intensity was one hour per week and the median magnitude was 4.5 hours. The mean age of participants in the samples was 11.3 years and the mean percentage of males was 48.1%. Out of the 23 studies, 20 of them included pretest measurements. With regards to the methodological quality of the studies, the mean score obtained with the quality scale (range: 0–8) was 6.1 (minimum: 3.4, maximum: 7.5). The results of the critical appraisal for the selected studies are presented in Additional File4. As all of the studies were carried out in schools, it was not possible to randomly assign the subjects to the experimental conditions, but in all of them the decision regarding which group received the intervention or the control condition was at random, with the exception of one study[41]. Only in three studies[20, 23, 40] was an active control group used, with the remaining studies using a nonactive one. In eight studies[21, 29, 30, 5256] there was attrition in the experimental group and all of them reported intent-to-treat analyses, with the exception of one study[56]. In 15 studies[1921, 23, 2730, 33, 34, 40, 5255] the assessor was blinded. All of the studies assessed the subjects in the same conditions (e.g., at the same time), and four studies[22, 24, 40, 56] did not report the reliability of the measurement instruments used. Only two studies were unpublished papers[33, 34]. The most frequent profession for the first author was physiotherapist (18 studies) and the studies were carried out between 1984 and 2011.

Table 1 Characteristics of the studies included in the meta-analysis

Mean effect size and heterogeneity analysis

The main measure of treatment effectiveness was the effect size obtained in the posttest and in the follow-up for the outcome measures of behaviours and knowledge. Separate meta-analyses were carried out for each combination of outcome measure and time point. Table 2 shows the main results for the four meta-analyses, and Figures 1,2,3,4 present a forest plot for each one of them. Overall, the four average effect sizes were positive in favour of the treatments (Table 2). Furthermore, all of the mean effect sizes were of a large magnitude according to the Cohen’s criteria[57], as they were over or close to 0.8.

Table 2 Mean effect size and heterogeneity analysis for the two outcome measures in the posttest and follow-up.
Figure 1
figure 1

Forest plot of effect sizes for measures of behaviours in the posttest.

Figure 2
figure 2

Forest plot of effect sizes for measures of knowledge in the posttest.

Figure 3
figure 3

Forest plot of effect sizes for measures of behaviours in the follow-up.

Figure 4
figure 4

Forest plot of effect sizes for measures of knowledge in the follow-up.

Figure 1 presents a forest plot for the behaviour measures in the posttest, with a mean effect size of d + = 1.33 (95% CI: 0.76 and 1.90), statistically significant and with the effect sizes exhibiting a large variability (I 2 = 97%). As Figure 1 shows, one of the studies exhibited an outlying effect size of d = 13.033[20]. The reasons for this so different result in comparison to the remaining studies included in the meta-analysis can be found in the characteristics of the intervention implemented. Thus, out of the 23 studies, this one was who exhibited the longest intensity (2.4 hours per week), the largest magnitude (a total of 19 hours of intervention), the only one that included homework; in addition, this study was one of the three that used family cotherapists, and one of the eight that used teachers as cotherapists. A more representative estimate of the treatment effectiveness for the set of studies included in this meta-analysis was obtained by removing this study from the analysis. When this study was removed, the mean effect size decreased to d + = 0.89 (95% CI: 0.39 and 1.38), although still being statistically significant and remaining a large heterogeneity (I 2 = 96%). Thus, the inclusion of this study in the analyses implied an increase of 49.4% for the mean effect size (from 0.89 to 1.33). The extremely atypical effect size obtained in this study advises, therefore, to remove it from the moderator analyses.

Figure 2 presents a forest plot for knowledge measures in the posttest, with a mean effect size of d + = 1.29 (95% CI: 0.90 and 1.68), statistically significant and with a large magnitude. The 16 studies also showed a large variability (I 2 = 96%).

Twelve studies enabled us to calculate effect sizes from the follow-ups, being the range between two and 96 months, and with a mean of 16.2 months and a median of 11 months. Out of these, six studies reported measures of behaviours. Figure 3 presents the forest plot, with a mean effect size of d + = 1.80 (95% CI: 0.67 and 2.92), statistically significant and even larger than that obtained in the posttest. However, the Méndez and Gómez’s (2001) study[20] showed a very outlying effect size of d = 12.957. By deleting this study from the analysis, the mean effect decreased to d + = 0.44 and it did not reach the statistical significance (95% CI: -0.41 and 1.28). This estimate of the true effect of the interventions seems to be more representative of the set of studies included in the meta-analysis.

Nine studies assessed knowledge in the follow-up. Figure 4 presents the forest plot, with a mean effect size of d + = 0.76 (95% CI: 0.47 and 1.05), statistically significant and exhibiting a large heterogeneity (I 2 = 81%). Two studies[20, 23] exhibited an effect size of d = 1.709, slightly over those obtained in the remaining studies. When these two effect sizes were removed from the analysis, the mean effect size decreased to d + = 0.55, although maintaining the statistical significance (95% CI: 0.34 and 0.76) and still exhibiting a large heterogeneity (I 2 = 58%).

Analyzing publication bias

Due to the low representativeness of the unpublished studies in our meta-analysis, we checked whether publication bias could be a threat to the validity of our results by applying an ANOVA to compare the mean effect size of the published vs. unpublished studies for the outcome variable of knowledge in the posttest and in the follow-up. In the posttest, the ANOVA reached a statistically significant difference between the mean effect sizes for the published and unpublished studies [Q B(1) = 5.86, p = .015; R 2 = 0.541], but contrary to the expectations: a higher effect size was found in the unpublished (d + = 2.093) than in the published studies (d + = 1.127). In the follow-up, the ANOVA did not reach the statistical significance [Q B(1) = 0.02, p = .879; R 2 = 0.0], although with the mean effects showing the same trend than in the posttest (d + = 0.755 and 0.829 for the published and unpublished studies, respectively).

The scarce representativeness of the unpublished studies recovered in our meta-analysis led us to examine further whether publication bias might be a threat against our meta-analytic results. With this purpose, a funnel plot was constructed for each one of the four meta-analyses, and the trim-and-fill method[46] was applied, when needed, in order to achieve symmetry in the funnel plot by imputing effect sizes. Figure 5 presents the funnel plot for the effect sizes obtained with measures of behaviours in the posttest. The trim-and-fill method had to impute two new effect sizes (on the left side of the graph) to achieve a symmetric funnel plot. Adding these two adjusted effect sizes led to a decrease of the mean effect, from the original d + = 1.33 (see Figure 1) to a d + = 0.74 (95% CI: 0.11 and 1.36), that is, a decrease of 44%.

Figure 5
figure 5

Funnel plot of effect sizes for measures of behaviours in the posttest. Full circles are imputed effect sizes from the Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim-and-fill method to achieve symmetry in the funnel plot.

Figure 6 presents the funnel plot for measures of knowledge in the posttest. The trim-and-fill method had to impute six new effect sizes to achieve symmetry in the funnel plot, giving rise to a decrease in the mean effect from the original d + = 1.29 (see Figure 2) to a d + = 0.75 (95% CI: 0.31 and 1.19), that is, a decrease of 39.5%. In the follow-up, as shown in Figure 7 and Figure 8 for measures of behaviours and knowledge, respectively, the funnel plot did not depart from symmetry and the trim-and-fill did not have to impute any effect size. Therefore, these analyses point towards the potential existence of publication bias in our meta-analyses in the posttest, but not in those for the follow-up.

Figure 6
figure 6

Funnel plot of effect sizes for measures of knowledge in the posttest. Full circles are imputed effect sizes from the Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim-and-fill method to achieve symmetry in the funnel plot.

Figure 7
figure 7

Funnel plot of effect sizes for measures of behaviours in the follow-up. The absence of full circles in the graph indicates an approximately symmetric funnel plot.

Figure 8
figure 8

Funnel plot of effect sizes for measures of knowledge in the follow-up. The absence of full circles in the graph indicates a symmetric funnel plot.

Analyzing moderator variables

In the four meta-analyses, the effect sizes exhibited a large heterogeneity (based on the Q statistics and the I 2 indices; see Figures 1,2,3,4), supporting our decision of applying random-effects models. In order to determine which moderator variables were influencing the effect sizes, ANOVAs (for the qualitative variables) and simple meta-regressions (for the quantitative variables) were carried out. The analysis of the moderator variables was applied for the two meta-analyses that included 10 or more studies: behaviours and knowledge in the posttest. In these analyses, the outlying effect size obtained in the Méndez and Gómez’s (2001) study[20] was removed from the statistical analyses for measures of behaviours in the posttest.

Outcome variable: behaviours in the posttest

Several treatment characteristics were coded in the studies. Tables 3 and4 present the results of the ANOVAs and meta-regressions, respectively, to examine the influence of moderator variables on the effect sizes. As Table 3 shows, the type of treatment did not reach a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes (p = .525). The majority of the treatments (11 studies) applied postural hygiene (PH) alone, whereas the combination of PH with physiotherapy exercise and PH with physical activity were represented in the analyses with one study only each one. Thus, a so unbalanced distribution of the studies limits the scope of these results. When the studies were classified as a function of the type of postural hygiene, a statistically significant relationship was found with the effect sizes (p = .046), with better results exhibited by combining acquisition of knowledge (AK) with posture training habits (PTH; d + = 1.466: 95% CI: 0.715 and 2.217) in comparison to AK alone (d + = −0.126) and the combination of AK with PTH and stimulation of dynamic postures (d + = 0.820). In fact, only the combination of AK with PTH showed a mean effect size statistically significant. Similarly, when analyzing the teaching mode of postural hygiene, statistically significant differences were found (p = .010) when comparing theoretical teaching (TT) alone (d + = 0.051) with the combination of TT plus practical teaching (d + = 1.378). In particular, the combination of TT plus practical teaching showed a statistically significant mean effect, whereas the mean effect for TT alone was practically null. Other qualitative variables related to the treatment characteristics that did not reach a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes were (see Table 3) the use of external agents (p = .393), the use of parents as paraprofessionals (p = .688), the use of teachers as paraprofessionals (p = .383), and the mode of application of the treatment (p = .594).

Table 3 Results of the mixed-effects ANOVAs for the qualitative moderator variables on the effect sizes obtained from measures of behaviours in the posttest
Table 4 Results of the mixed-effects meta-regressions for the continuous moderator variables on the effect sizes obtained from measures of behaviours in the posttest

With regards to the continuous variables, the magnitude of the intervention (p < .05) showed a positive and statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes, and the intensity of the intervention approached to statistical significance (p = .06). Thus, the larger the intensity and the total number of hours of intervention, the better the effectiveness of the treatment. The duration of the treatment did not show a statistical relationship with the effect sizes (see Table 4).

Table 4 also presents the results for two moderator variables related to the participant characteristics in the samples: the mean age and the gender distribution (percentage of males). Anyone of them reached a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes, although the negative value of their slopes (b j = −0.662 and −0.033, respectively) indicated a slight trend to show lower effect sizes as the mean age and the percentage of males increased.

With regards to the methodological characteristics, Table 3 presents the results of applying ANOVAs on several qualitative variables. Statistically significant differences (p = .008; R 2 = 0.062) were found when comparing the mean effect obtained by studies that used blinded evaluators (d + = 1.310) to those that did not use them (d + = −0.125), the first category exhibiting a statistically significant mean effect size. Neither the use of pretest measures (p = .702) nor the type of control group (p = .688) showed a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes. However, a trend was found to exhibit lower effect sizes when the treatments were compared to active control groups (d + = 0.642) than when using nonactive controls (d + = 0.931). Simple meta-regressions were applied on two continuous methodological variables: the differential attrition between the treatment and control groups and the total score obtained in the methodological quality scale (see Table 4). The methodological quality showed a positive, statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes (p < .05, R 2 = 0.035), whereas the differential attrition did not show a statistical relationship.

Finally, the publication date did not reach a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes (see Table 4). A thorough examination of the studies revealed the existence of only a few research teams producing the majority of the studies included in the meta-analysis. The existence of a statistical relationship between research teams and effect sizes can limit the generalizability of the meta-analytic results. With this purpose, an ANOVA was applied once the studies were classified as a function of the research team. As Table 5 shows, a statistically significant relationship was found between research team and effect size (p = .002), with the largest mean effect obtained by the Gómez, Méndez et al.’s team (d + = 3.145), followed by the Cardon, Geldhof et al.’s team (d + = 1.504), and with the other two research teams (USA and South Korea) showing a mean effect statistically not significant (d + = −0.463 and 0.205, respectively). When the outlying effect size obtained in the Méndez and Gómez’s (2001) study[20] was removed from the analysis, still a statistically significant relationship was found between research team and effect size (p = .035), but in this case only the Belgium team exhibited a mean effect size statistically significant.

Table 5 Results of the mixed-effects ANOVA for the qualitative moderator variable “research team” on the effect sizes obtained from measures of behaviours in the posttest

Outcome variable: knowledge in the posttest

Sixteen studies enabled us to obtain an effect size for measures of knowledge in the posttest. The ANOVAs and meta-regressions applied to search for potential moderator variables are shown in Tables 6 and7, respectively. The majority of the studies applied interventions based on postural hygiene (PH) alone (12 studies; d + = 1.301), whereas only three combined PH with physiotherapy exercise (d + = 1.521), and one combined PH with physical activity (d + = 0.537). The comparison of the three mean effect sizes did not reach a statistical significant result (p = .583), although the combination of PH with physiotherapy exercise exhibited the largest mean effect. Neither the type of postural hygiene (p = .159), nor the teaching method of postural hygiene (p = .669), nor the use of external agents (p = .201), nor the use of the parents as paraprofessionals (p = .384) reached a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes. When the studies were classified as a function of whether they used or not teachers as cotherapists, a statistically significant difference was found (p = .009; R 2 = 0.497) in favour of the interventions that did not used them (d + = 0.730 and 1.544). The mode of application of the treatment influenced the results (p = .004; R 2 = 0.481), with the best results for the interventions directly applied by the therapists (d + = 1.578), followed by mixed interventions (d + = 0.534), that is, interventions where part of the treatment was applied by cotherapists that were family members or teachers who had been trained by the therapist, and part was applied directly by the therapist. The treatment duration showed a negative and statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes (p < .01; R 2 = 0.430), suggesting that the lower the number of weeks of treatment, the better the results obtained (see Table 7). However, two studies had very extreme treatment durations (96 weeks both of them)[21, 30] in comparison to the remaining studies (range: 1 to 15 weeks). When these two studies were removed from the analysis, the treatment duration did not show a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes Q R(1) = 0.77, p > .05]. On the other hand, neither the intensity nor the magnitude of the interventions were statistically related to the effect sizes.

Table 6 Results of the mixed-effects ANOVAs for the qualitative moderator variables on the effect sizes obtained from measures of knowledge in the posttest
Table 7 Results of the mixed-effects meta-regressions for the continuous moderator variables on the effect sizes obtained from measures of knowledge in the posttest

With regards to the participant characteristics, simple meta-regressions applied on the mean age (in years) of the samples and on the percentage of males did not show a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes (see Table 7). Similar results were found when the influence of methodological variables was tested: neither the type of control group (p = .198), nor the use of blinded evaluators (p = .553), nor the differential attrition, nor the quality score showed a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes (see Tables 6 and7).

Finally, two extrinsic variables were analyzed: the publication year and the research team. As Table 7 shows, the publication year did not show a statistically significant relationship with the effect sizes. With regards to the research team, a highly statistically significant result was obtained (p < .001) with a large proportion of variance accounted for (R 2 = 0.718). The Gómez, Méndez et al.’s team exhibited the largest mean effect (d + = 2.043) and the Kovacs et al.’s team the lowest one (d + = 0.289) (see Table 8).

Table 8 Results of the mixed-effects ANOVA for the qualitative moderator variable “research team” on the effect sizes obtained from measures of knowledge in the posttest

Discussion

The main objective of our study was to determine the effectiveness of preventive physiotherapy treatments for back care in children and adolescents, as well as to examine the treatment, subject, context, methodological and extrinsic characteristics that may be moderating the results. A total of 23 studies met our selection criteria and standardized mean differences were calculated from each of them by comparing a treatment to a control group. Separate meta-analyses were carried out for effect sizes obtained from measures of behaviours and knowledge, both in the posttest and in the follow-up. Moderator analyses were carried out for behaviours and knowledge in the posttest.

Relating to behaviours in the posttest

Although the mean effect size obtained for measures of behaviours in the posttest was d + = 1.33, when an outlying effect size was removed from the analysis[20], the mean effect decreased to d + = 0.89, although still being highly statistically significant. The evidence of an asymmetric funnel plot led to suspect that publication bias might be a threat for the meta-analytic results. So, the trim-and-fill method gave a more conservative estimate of the true effect of preventive interventions for LBP, with a mean effect of d + = 0.74. From the results obtained in the analysis of the treatment modalities used, the type of postural hygiene seems to be a relevant moderator of effect size, with the combination of knowledge acquisition plus posture training habits being the most efficacious. The teaching method of postural hygiene also influenced the effect sizes, with better results when theoretical and practical teachings were combined. The hypothesis that the duration, intensity and magnitude of the treatment influence the results has been partially confirmed by our results, enabling us to conclude that, the higher the intensity and magnitude, the more efficacious the treatment. Previous research have shown that the interventions improve their benefits when they include the figures of parents or teachers as cotherapists[20, 27]. However, our hypotheses on the positive influence of using external agents and parents and teachers as paraprofessionals were not supported by our results. Regarding the participant characteristics in the samples, the hypothesis that the age of subjects would negatively influence the results was not supported. In the same vein, the gender distribution of the sample did not influence the results, not supporting our hypothesis. Our hypothesis that studies with a nonactive control group would have higher effect sizes than those with an active control group was not confirmed, although the results pointed in that direction. This result must be interpreted very cautiously because only a few studies used an active control group[20, 23, 40].

Relating to knowledge in the posttest

For knowledge acquisition a positive and highly statistically significant mean effect size was obtained, d + = 1.29, although the trim-and-fill method had to impute six new effect sizes to achieve symmetry in the funnel plot. Therefore, a more conservative estimate of the true effect that controls for publication bias is d + = 0.75, still statistically significant and of a large magnitude. The different treatment modalities did not seem to affect the effect sizes. The hypothesis that the duration, intensity and magnitude of the treatment may influence the results was not supported by our results. Using external agents and the presence of family paraprofessionals did not influence the effect size magnitude. However the presence of teachers as cotherapists did affect the results, but inversely to our expectations. The mode of application influenced the effect size magnitude, with direct interventions obtaining the best results. Regarding the participant characteristics, neither the gender nor the age of the subjects influenced the results. In the case of gender, Rebolho[58] has shown a higher level of knowledge acquisition for females than for males. With regards to the type of control group, this variable did not influence the results, and therefore we could not confirm our hypothesis of a lower effect size for studies that compare the intervention with an active control group than when the control group is nonactive.

Results in the follow-up

The maintenance of the changes due to the interventions was assessed in a few studies that enabled us to calculate effect sizes in the follow-up. With regards to measures of behaviours, the mean effect size was positive and of a large magnitude, d + = 1.80. However, when the extremely outlying effect size obtained in the Méndez and Gómez’s (2001)[20] study was removed from the analysis, the mean effect decreased to d + = 0.44 and did not reach the statistical significance. In the case of measures of knowledge, the mean effect size was of large magnitude and statistically significant, d + = 0.76. Therefore, with regards to behaviour measures we cannot be sure that the benefits of the interventions will be maintained over time.

Limitations of the meta-analysis

It is important to note some limitations of our meta-analysis. The absence of a more detailed description in the primary studies about such important characteristics as the treatment techniques, its mode of application, caused us uncertainty in our coding process. On the other hand, the small number of studies in our meta-analysis makes our results be interpreted with caution and be taken as provisional, pending the publication of new studies in this field. In addition, the limited number of studies has prevented to formulate an explanatory model of the effect sizes variability, by applying a multiple meta-regression model. Another limitation is the evidence of publication bias in some of our meta-analytic results, inviting us to a cautious interpretation of the results and to take the effect estimates obtained with the trim-and-fill method as more appropriate. Finally, a circumstance that limits the generalizability of our results is the scarce number of research teams that have carried out the studies included in our meta-analysis.

Implications for clinical practice

The main implication of our results for clinical practice is that preventive physiotherapy interventions for back care should combine knowledge and training of postural habits with physiotherapy exercises.

Implications for future research

The results of our meta-analysis allow us to propose some recommendations for future research in this field. Firstly, it is advisable that future studies report more information regarding the characteristics of the treatments applied. Furthermore, with the purpose of obtaining important data relating to the maintenance of the changes, researchers should conduct follow-ups. One of the 23 studies[20] exhibited a very large effect size in measures of behaviours, both in the posttest (d = 13.033) and in the follow-up (d = 12.957). This study was who exhibited the longest intensity (2.4 hours per week), the largest magnitude (a total of 19 hours of intervention), the only one that included homework and, in addition, it used family and teachers as cotherapists. In addition, this study achieved the maximum quality score out of all of the studies of the meta-analysis. Although this study is not representative of the set of studies included in the meta-analysis, the extremely large effectiveness found in it advises that new studies try to replicate their results by implementing interventions similar to that applied in this study.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the combined treatment of postural hygiene with physiotherapy exercise has been proven to be the most efficacious in relation to the two outcome variables: behaviours and knowledge. The treatments were successful in significantly increasing the behaviours and knowledge acquired both in the posttest and in follow-up.

References

  1. Bejia I, Abid N, Ben Salem K, Letaief M, Younes M, Touzi M, Bergaoui N: Low back pain in a cohort of 622 Tunisian schoolchildren and adolescents: an epidemiological study. Eur Spine J. 2005, 14: 331-336. 10.1007/s00586-004-0785-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Feldman DE, Shrier I, Rossignol M, Abenhaim L: Risk factors for the development of low back pain in adolescence. Am J Epidemiol. 2001, 154: 30-36. 10.1093/aje/154.1.30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Balagué F, Dutoit G, Waldburger M: Low back pain in schoolchildren. An epidemiological study. Scand J Rehabil Med. 1988, 20: 175-179.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Szpalski M, Gunzburg R, Balagué F, Nordin M, Mélot C: A 2-year prospective longitudinal study on low back pain in primary school children. Eur Spine J. 2002, 11: 459-464. 10.1007/s00586-002-0385-y.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Watson KD, Papageorgiou AC, Jones GT, Taylor S, Symmons DP, Silman AJ, Macfarlane GJ: Low back pain in schoolchildren: occurrence and characteristics. Pain. 2002, 97: 87-92. 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00008-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Harreby M, Nygaard B, Jessen T, Larsen E, Storr-Paulsen A, Lindahl A, Fisker I, Laegaard E: Risk factors for low back pain in a cohort of 1389 Danish school children: an epidemiologic study. Eur Spine J. 1999, 8: 444-450. 10.1007/s005860050203.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Masiero S, Carraro E, Celia A, Sarto D, Ermani M: Prevalence of nonspecific low back pain in schoolchildren aged between 13 and 15 years. Acta Paediatr. 2008, 97: 212-216. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00603.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Fairbank JC, Pynsent PB, Van Poortvliet JA, Phillips H: Influence of anthropometric factors and joint laxity in the incidence of adolescent back pain. Spine. 1984, 9: 461-464. 10.1097/00007632-198407000-00007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Trevelyan FC, Legg SJ: Back pain in school children–where to from here?. Appl Ergon. 2006, 37: 45-54. 10.1016/j.apergo.2004.02.008.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Jones GT, Watson KD, Silman AJ, Symmons DP, Macfarlane GJ: Predictors of low back pain in British schoolchildren: a population-based prospective cohort study. Pediatrics. 2003, 111: 822-828. 10.1542/peds.111.4.822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Cakmak A, Yücel B, Ozyalçn SN, Bayraktar B, Ural HI, Duruöz MT, Genç A: The frequency and associated factors of low back pain among a younger population in Turkey. Spine. 2004, 29: 1567-1572. 10.1097/01.BRS.0000131432.72531.96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Linton SJ, van Tulder MW: Preventive interventions for back and neck pain problems: what is the evidence?. Spine. 2001, 26: 778-787. 10.1097/00007632-200104010-00019.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Tveito TH, Hysing M, Eriksen HR: Low back pain interventions at the workplace: a systematic literature review. Occup Med. 2004, 54: 3-13. 10.1093/occmed/kqg109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Gatty CM, Turner M, Buitendorp DJ, Batman H: The effectiveness of back pain and injury prevention programs in the workplace. Work. 2003, 20: 257-266.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Albaladejo C, Kovacs FM, Royuela A, del Pino R, Zamora J, Spanish Back Pain Research Network: The efficacy of a short education program and a short physiotherapy program for treating low back pain in primary care: a cluster randomized trial. Spine. 2010, 35: 483-496. 10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181b9c9a7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Cardon G, Balagué F: Low back pain prevention’s effects in schoolchildren. What is the evidence?. Eur Spine J. 2004, 13: 663-679. 10.1007/s00586-004-0749-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. European Region of the World Confederation of Physical therapist (ER-WCPT): Pain in the back. Avoiding back pain in children and teenagers. 2010, [web site]. April 21, 2009. Available atwww.painintheback.eu/home.html.Accessed,

    Google Scholar 

  18. Sheldon MR: Lifting instruction to children in an elementary school. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther. 1994, 19: 105-110.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Cardon G, De Clercq D, De Bourdeaudhuij I: Effects of back care education in elementary schoolchildren. Acta Paediatr. 2000, 89: 1010-1017. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2000.tb00426.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Méndez FJ, Gómez-Conesa A: Postural hygiene program to prevent low back pain. Spine. 2001, 26: 1280-1286. 10.1097/00007632-200106010-00022.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Geldhof E, Cardon G, De Bourdeaudhuij I, De Clercq D: Effects of a two-school-year multifactorial back education program in elementary schoolchildren. Spine. 2006, 31: 1965-1973. 10.1097/01.brs.0000228722.12968.d2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Spence SM, Jensen GM, Shepard KF: Comparison of methods of teaching children proper lifting techniques. Phys Ther. 1984, 64: 1055-1061.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Gómez-Conesa A, Méndez FX: Programa escolar de educación para la salud en el cuidado de la espalda [Schoolchild Health Education program for back care]. Rev Iberoam Fisioter Kinesiol. 2000, 3: 74-83.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Vidal J, Cantallops J, Borrás PA, Ponseti FX, Palou P: Educació per a la salut: la prevenció del mal d’esquena en el context escolar. [Education for health: the prevention of back pain in the school context]. Anuari de l’Educació de les Illes Balears. 2009, 424-436.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Robertson HC, Lee V: Effects of back care lessons on sitting and lifting by primary students. Aust J Physiother. 1990, 36: 245-248.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Feingold AJ, Jacobs K: The effect of education on backpack wearing and posture in a middle school population. Work. 2002, 18: 287-294.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Cardon G, De Bourdeaudhuij I, De Clercq D: Back care education in elementary school: a pilot study investigating the complementary role of the class teacher. Patient Educ Couns. 2001, 45: 219-226. 10.1016/S0738-3991(01)00122-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Cardon G, De Bourdeaudhuij I, De Clercq D: Knowledge and perceptions about back education among elementary school students, teachers, and parents in Belgium. J Sch Health. 2002, 72: 100-106. 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2002.tb06524.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Cardon GM, De Clercq DL, De Bourdeaudhuij IM: Back education efficacy in elementary schoolchildren: a 1-year follow-up study. Spine. 2002, 27: 299-305. 10.1097/00007632-200202010-00020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Cardon GM, de Clercq DL, Geldhof EJ, Verstraete S, de Bourdeaudhuij IM: Back education in elementary schoolchildren: the effects of adding a physical activity promotion program to a back care program. Eur Spine J. 2007, 16: 125-133. 10.1007/s00586-006-0095-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Vicas-Kunse P: Educating our children: the pilot school program. Occup Med. 1992, 7: 173-177.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Zapater AR, Silveira DM, de Vitta A, Padovani CR, Plácido da Silva JC: Postura sentada: a eficácia de um programa de educação para escolares [Seat posture: the efficiency of an educational program for scholars]. Ciênc Saúde Coletiva. 2004, 9: 191-199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Cardoso C: Intervención para el cuidado de la espalda en niños y adolescentes brasileños. [Intervention for back care in Brazilian children and adolescents] PhD thesis. 2009, University of Murcia (Spain), Faculty of Medicine

    Google Scholar 

  34. Martínez M: Intervención en higiene postural y cuidado de la espalda en escolares. [Postural hygiene intervention and back care in schoolchildren. Evaluation and effect] PhD thesis. 2007, University of Murcia (Spain), Faculty of Medicine

    Google Scholar 

  35. Lipsey MW: Identifying interesting variables and analysis opportunities. The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. Edited by: Cooper HM, Hedges LV, Valentine JC. 2009, Sage, New York, 147-158. 2

    Google Scholar 

  36. van Tulder M, Furlan A, Bombardier C, Bouter L, Editorial Board of the Cochrane Collaboration Back Review Group: Updated method guidelines for systematic reviews in the Cochrane Collaboration back review group. Spine. 2003, 28: 1290-1299.

  37. Orwin RG, Vevea JL: Evaluating coding decisions. The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. Edited by: Cooper HM, Hedges LV, Valentine JC. 2009, Sage, New York, 177-203. 2

    Google Scholar 

  38. Borenstein MJ, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR: Introduction to meta-analysis. 2009, Wiley, Chichester. UK

    Book  Google Scholar 

  39. Morris SB: Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organizational Research Methods. 2008, 11: 364-386. 10.1177/1094428106291059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Gómez-Conesa A, Méndez FX: Ergonomía en las actividades de vida diaria en la infancia [Ergonomy in childhood daily life activities]. Fisioterapia. 2000, 22: 130-142.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Park JH, Kim JS: Effects of spinal health educational programs for elementary school children. J Spec Pediatr Nurs. 2011, 16: 121-129. 10.1111/j.1744-6155.2011.00278.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Higgins JPT, Green S: Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. 2011, Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011], The Cochrane Collaboration, Available at:www.cochrane-handbook.org,

    Google Scholar 

  43. Sánchez-Meca J, Marín-Martínez F: International encyclopedia of education Volume 7 3rd ed. 2010, Meta-analysis. In: Peterson P, Baker E, McGaw B, ed, Oxford. Elsevier, 274-282.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  44. Glass G, McGaw B, Smith ML: Meta-analysis in social research. 1981, Sage, Newburry Park, Ca

    Google Scholar 

  45. Sánchez-Meca J, Marín-Martínez F: Confidence intervals for the overall effect size in random-effects meta-analysis. Psychol Meth. 2008, 13: 31-48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Duval S, Tweedie R: Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. 2000, 56: 455-463. 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Raudenbush SW: Analyzing effect sizes: Random-effects models. The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis. Edited by: Cooper HM, Hedges LV, Valentine JC. 2009, Sage, New York, 295-315. 2

    Google Scholar 

  48. SPSS 15.0: Statistical Package for the Social Sciences. 2006, vers. 15.00, SPSS, Inc

    Google Scholar 

  49. Review Manager (RevMan) [Computer program]: Version 5.0. Copenhagen. 2008, The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration

    Google Scholar 

  50. Borenstein MJ, Hedges LV, Higgins JPT, Rothstein HR: Comprehensive meta-analysis 2.0. 2005, Englewood, NJ, Biostat Inc

    Google Scholar 

  51. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JPA, Clarke M, Devereaux PJ, Kleijnen J, Moher D: The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. J Clin Epidemiol. 2009, 62: c1-c34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Dolphens M, Cagnie B, Danneels L, De Clercq D, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Cardon G: Long-term effectiveness of a back education programme in elementary schoolchildren: an 8-year follow-up study. Eur Spine J. 2011, 20: 2134-2142. 10.1007/s00586-011-1856-9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Geldhof E, Cardon G, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Danneels L, Coorevits P, Vanderstraeten G, De Clercq D: Effects of back posture education on elementary schoolchildren’s back function. Eur Spine J. 2007, 16: 829-839. 10.1007/s00586-006-0199-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Geldhof E, Cardon G, De Bourdeaudhuij I, De Clercq D: Back posture education in elementary schoolchildren: a 2-year follow-up study. Eur Spine J. 2007, 16: 841-850. 10.1007/s00586-006-0227-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Geldhof E, Cardon G, De Bourdeaudhuij I, De Clercq D: Back posture education in elementary schoolchildren: stability of two-year intervention effects. Eura Medicophys. 2007, 43: 369-379.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Kovacs F, Oliver-Frontera M, Plana MN, Royuela A, Muriel A, Gestoso M, the Spanish Back Pain Research Network: Improving schoolchildren's knowledge of methods for the prevention and management of low back pain: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Spine. 2011, 36: E505-512. 10.1097/BRS.0b013e3181dccebc.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Cohen J: Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 1988, Hillsdale: NJ, Erlbaun, 2

    Google Scholar 

  58. Rebolho MC: Efeitos da educação postural nas mudanças de hábitos em escolares das 1ª a 4ª séries do ensino fundamental. [Effects of posture education on the habit changes of 1st to 4th grades elementary school students] PhD thesis. 2005, University of São Paulo (Brazil), Faculty of Medicine

    Google Scholar 

Pre-publication history

Download references

Acknowledgements

No funding was received for this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Inmaculada Calvo-Muñoz.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

All authors contributed to conception and design, acquisition, analysis and interpretation of data and drafting of the manuscript. AGC and JSM participated in the critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content. ICM and JSM performed statistical analyses. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Calvo-Muñoz, I., Gómez-Conesa, A. & Sánchez-Meca, J. Preventive physiotherapy interventions for back care in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. BMC Musculoskelet Disord 13, 152 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2474-13-152

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2474-13-152

Keywords